Over the last few decades there has been a huge swing towards less punitive methods of dog training.
Watching a modern trainer in action is a very different experience from watching old school traditionalists. Gone are the barked commands, the emphasis on ‘respect’ or ‘dominance’ and even intimidation.
In many cases the use of punishment has been entirely replaced by the use of food and games. But hang on a moment! Does treat training actually work, or are we sacrificing training standards by relying so heavily on rewards?
Is dog training with treats effective?
What if we don’t want to wave food around or to ‘beg’ or ‘plead’ with our dogs to come when we call them? What if we want reliable results and we don’t want to wait around for months or years before we get them.
Are we being swept along in the latest ‘fad’ or ‘craze’? Isn’t this just a passing fashion? A scam even. Because let’s face it, how are we going to control our dogs when we run out of treats?
Fortunately, the answer is yes, reward based dog training is effective. But this article isn’t about telling you what I think, it’s about showing you what others are achieving.
Because at the end of the day, it’s results that count right? So we are going to look at the evidence. And at the people who are achieving great results without ever resorting to punishment.
Dog training in the real world
Positive reinforcement training is based on behavioral science. But training a dog in a living environment is not the same as studying rats in a laboratory. So how do we know that these modern science based methods work in the real world?
This is important, and it’s a question that comes up again and again. So we’ll start by looking at dogs that are carrying out important jobs in distracting environments.
Guide dog training methods
When you see a guide dog leading their blind handler safely across a busy road, threading their way through a crowded street or along a woodland path, ignoring squirrels, ball games, friendly dogs and all kinds of other distractions, you are watching a dog trained using positive reinforcement.
The Guide Dogs for the Blind Association did not embrace positive reinforcement training to be a part of the ‘in crowd’ or to be ‘cool’, they did so because it works.
In fact, it works so well that the USA guide dogs organisation reported an increase in pass rate from 50% (the old rate) to 80% in their dogs after switching to modern methods in 2005. In addition to a significant decrease in handler training time.
Bomb disposal dog training methods
What about explosives detection dogs? One false move could be fatal for handler and dog. How are these dogs trained?
Once again, these dogs are trained to carry out their important jobs using positive reinforcement. You can read more about them via that link.
Search and rescue dog training methods
You will have seen search and rescue dogs on TV attending earthquake zones and the aftermath of terrible natural disasters.
These dogs have to work in often appalling conditions under all kinds of distraction. And how are they trained? The answer is with toys, games, and food.
Are we starting to see a bit of a pattern here?
The fact is, almost all service industries that utilise trained dogs, now train their dogs with positive reinforcement. From the military, to the police, from medical detection dogs to dogs that hunt down bed bugs, diseases, or drugs. Gone are the days of pinning, dominating and punishing these important dogs. They are now trained without force.
Getting great results from your dog training method
Many of these services are run by charities, small businesses, government agencies or large commercial corporations.
These organisations are not interested in ‘fads’ and the latest fashion. They are interested in results.
The same pattern of changing to science based training methods has taken place throughout the world of dog sports, from the obedience ring, to working trials and agility. With one notable exception. And we’ll look at that in a moment.
So what prompted this change in training methods?
The reason that these organisations leading the way in dog training all switched to positive reinforcement training is because they ran trials or pilot schemes and found that the new training methods worked faster, were more effective, and saved money.
The reason that they ran the trials in the first place was due to the weight of emerging scientific evidence (see resources below) and because they were encouraged to do so by the behavioral scientists that advise them.
Positive reinforcement training is not new, and has been studied in laboratories for the best part of a century.
It is based on operant conditioning, or the way in which animals learn through the consequences of their behavior. Moving from laboratory to field, or real world training took some time.
Several leading behaviorists and animal trainers, such as Bob Bailey, Marion Breland, and Karen Pryor were involved in pioneering positive reinforcement training in animals outside of laboratory conditions.
There is some some excellent information on this topic on the late Sophia Yin’s website.
Scientific studies on dog training
If you are interested in science, you might like to look at some of the up to date scientific studies that have been influential in the movement towards positive reinforcement training. Quite a few studies, such as this one published in 2014 have shown that e-collar training has welfare implications for dogs. However, this won’t come as a surprise to many of you.
What is perhaps more interesting, is that there are measurable downsides to using much milder forms of punishment. And that’s what we’ll focus on here
Whilst much of our original evidence on positive reinforcement training is now quite old, more recently more studies focusing specifically on dogs have become available.
A study from 2004 showed that dogs trained with more rewards showed higher levels of obedience, and that dogs trained with more punishment exhibited more problem behaviors.
More recently in 2008. Emily Blackwell’s study showed that dogs trained using positive reinforcement were less likely to show aggression and fear than dogs trained using punitive methods.
Meghan Herron’s study in the same year, showed that all punishment was associated with higher levels of aggression and fear, even what many of us might consider mild aversives like ‘staring at a dog’ or making a growling noise at him.
A study in 2010 found a correlation between more frequent use of punishment and excitement/aggression.
Obviously aggression in dogs is undesirable but fear is a problem too, as it is associated with reduced ability to learn. So it’s not surprising that a 2011 study that looked at dogs in their own homes, found that positive reinforcement trained dogs were better at learning new skills and more interactive with their owners.
In 2017 a review of the literature was published in the Journal of Veterinary Behavior. It’s called The Effects Of Aversives In Dog Training and concludes that those working with or handling dogs should rely on positive reinforcement training techniques and avoid positive punishment and negative reinforcement as much as possible.
There are more studies listed in the resources below
How does positive dog training work?
It is quite common for people to think that they are already positive dog trainers. When in fact, they still use many different types of aversive in training.
By aversive – we mean anything which the dog doesn’t like. Aversives are simply tools for reducing unwanted behavior. They are what scientists also refer to as ‘punishers’. And the use of aversives is a form of punishment.
Punishment doesn’t have to cruel or violent. Intimidation, body blocking, growling, the use of rattle bottles etc. These are all forms of punishment. You can find more explanations on the behavioral meaning of punishment and reinforcement in the links below
Positive reinforcement training focuses on reinforcing behaviors through the delivery of desirable consequences – rewards like toys and games.
It avoids the use of punishment or what more traditional trainers often call corrections (consequences that the dog finds undesirable)
This may seem counterintuitive at first. Why not use rewards (for good behavior) and punishment (for bad behavior) and get the job done in half the time?
Why we don’t include punishment in modern dog training
Well, as we have seen, the evidence all points to punishment slowing down the learning process, and we can speculate on why that is.
It is clear from the studies available so far the fear is closely associated with punitive training methods, even when non physical punishment is involved. Punishment is also associated with aggression in dogs.
One of the main reasons that aversive free training probably works so well is that it prevents dogs from ‘freezing up’ through fear of consequences.
Mixing the two types of training (punishment and reward) might seem like a good idea, but as some of these studies have shown the history of the way the handler has treated the dog at any given point in time, will affect the way that the dog responds in the future (thus punishment in the past has an adverse effect in the future, even if the handler is now using rewards)
Studies have also shown that positive reinforcement is at least as effective if not more so, than techniques that rely on punishment. And this is important for those who are working with dogs in the presence of strong distractions.
Hunting dog training is a good example of such work
Hunting dog training methods
I talked earlier about an exception to the widespread uptake of positive reinforcement training, and that exception is retriever and spaniel training, my own particular interest and hobby.
The reasons for this are complex.
The hunting dog community has a long history of passing down skills as a ‘craft’ from one generation to the next and until recently has lacked the kind of structured training and grading system found in almost every other sport.
New hunting companion owners kind of muddled along, with no clear path to follow.
In addition, hunting dogs work in particularly distracting environments with period of boredom interspersed with periods of great excitement.
Unlike many service dogs, retrievers, hprs, and spaniels, work entirely off lead and in close contact with live game. This means that control of the consequences available to the dog (an essential part of positive reinforcement training) is more challenging.
However, forward thinking trainers are now beginning to realise the opportunities and benefits offered by positive reinforcement training, especially in basic training and working with puppies.
They are increasingly keen to work on the unique challenges facing positive reinforcement gundog trainers in the field. One by one, the stumbling blocks are being removed.
I mentioned earlier that learning theory gave us the ground rules for positive reinforcement training many years ago. Why has it only recently become popular?
The pace of change in dog training
Change does take time. Especially when many diverse groups of people are involved. And many of the actual studies done on dogs, like the ones mentioned above, are fairly recent.
It takes a while for information to filter down from researchers to field workers, and longer still for new techniques and principles to become widely adopted.
But positive reinforcement training is here now, to stay. It isn’t some cliquey minority that are teaching their dogs this way. There is a consensus amongst all canine behaviorists educated to degree level and beyond, that positive reinforcement training is the best, and most effective system for training our dogs.
This is true even in countries where e-collar training is still both legal and popular. The American Small Animal Veterinary Association for example, has placed a ‘position statement’ on dog training on its website
The simple fact is that positive reinforcement training has been widely adopted by mainstream dog trainers, in sports and canine service industries worldwide.
And the last bastions of resistance are crumbling.
Making the change from traditional to modern dog training
Most dog trainers/handlers/owners that have been training dogs for twenty years or more will have started out using punitive training methods. I did, as did all my contemporaries.
This is not something to beat yourself up over, or contemplate with regret. It was what it was. This is how dogs were trained.
The evidence now is convincing. There is a better way. And making the change has never been easier.
How about you? Have you made the switch yet? Share your thoughts below. I regularly update this page as more research is published, so let me know if you think I have missed anything.
References and further reading
E Fernandez 2024 The least inhibitive, functionally effective (LIFE) model: A new framework for ethical animal training practices. Journal Of Veterinary Behavior.
A Johnson & C Wynne 2024 Training Methods Used by Dog Guardians in the United States: Prevalence, Sources of Information, and Reasons for Use. Animals.
E N Feuerbacher et al. 2022 Give the dog a big bone: Magnitude but not delivery method of food impacts preference and reinforcer efficacy in dogs. American Psychological Association Psychnet
N J Hall et all 2021 Working Dog Training for the Twenty-First Century. Frontiers In Veterinary Science
R A Casey et al 2021. Dogs are more pessimistic if their owners use two or more aversive training methods. Scientific Reports 11.
L China et al 2020 Efficacy of Dog Training With and Without Remote Electronic Collars vs. a Focus on Positive Reinforcement. Frontiers In Veterinary Science
Vieira De Castro et al 2020 Does training method matter? Evidence for the negative impact of aversive-based methods on companion dog welfare. Plos One
L Lazarowski et al. 2020 Selecting Dogs for Explosives Detection: Behavioral Characteristics. Frontiers In Veterinary Science.
Vieira De Castro et al 2019. Carrot Versus Stick. The relationship between training methods and dog owner attachment. Applied Animal Behavior Science
Masson 2018 Electronic training devices: discussion on the pros and cons of their use in dogs
as a basis for the position statement of the European Society of Veterinary Clinical
Ethology (ESVCE). Journal of Veterinary Behavior
G Ziv 2017. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. Journal Of Veterinary Behavior
Cooper J et al 2014. The Welfare Consequences and Efficacy of Training Pet Dogs with Remote Electronic Training Collars in Comparison to Reward Based Training. PLOSone
Rooney, N & Cowan, S 2011. Training methods and owner–dog interactions: Links with dog behavior and learning ability. Applied Animal Behavior Science
Arhant, C et al 2010. Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 123, 131–142.
Herron, M et al 2009. Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing undesired behaviors. Applied Animal Behavior Science
Blackwell et al 2007. The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior
Hiby, E.F et al 2004. Dog training method: their use, effectiveness and interaction with behavior and welfare. Animal Welf
Free Training Tips
Get Pippa's free dog training tips delivered to your inbox